banner

News

Aug 22, 2023

Neurogenesis mediated plasticity is associated with reduced neuronal activity in CA1 during context fear memory retrieval

Scientific Reports volume 12, Article number: 7016 (2022) Cite this article

3105 Accesses

3 Citations

8 Altmetric

Metrics details

Postnatal hippocampal neurogenesis has been demonstrated to affect learning and memory in numerous ways. Several studies have now demonstrated that increased neurogenesis can induce forgetting of memories acquired prior to the manipulation of neurogenesis and, as a result of this forgetting can also facilitate new learning. However, the mechanisms mediating neurogenesis-induced forgetting are not well understood. Here, we used a subregion-based analysis of the immediate early gene c-Fos as well as in vivo fiber photometry to determine changes in activity corresponding with neurogenesis induced forgetting. We found that increasing neurogenesis led to reduced CA1 activity during context memory retrieval. We also demonstrate here that perineuronal net expression in areas CA1 is bidirectionally altered by the levels or activity of postnatally generated neurons in the dentate gyrus. These results suggest that neurogenesis may induce forgetting by disrupting perineuronal nets in CA1 which may otherwise protect memories from degradation.

The postnatal proliferation and integration of new neurons in the mammalian brain is a unique form of plasticity that has the potential to both create new connectivity as well as to disrupt existing circuits. As a consequence, there are numerous implications for postnatal neurogenesis as a modulator of hippocampal structure and function. Some recent theories suggest that postnatal neurogenesis is critical for shaping hippocampal circuits to optimize future learning1. Other previous work has demonstrated that, in many cases, elevated levels of neurogenesis positively impact processes such as learning2,3,4, memory5,6 and, cognitive flexibility4,7. Recent work has also shown that in addition to the positive influences that neurogenesis may have on learning and memory, there is also a weakening of memories that were formed prior to the elevation of neurogenesis8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15. Ultimately, this role of postnatal neurogenesis is also beneficial as it mitigates proactive interference between old and new memories. This occurs particularly in situations where there is conflict between the two memories, such as in the case of reversal learning. As a result, neurogenesis induced forgetting is beneficial for learning as it allows for faster acquisition of new memories10. This "retrograde" effect of neurogenesis impacts a wide variety of memory types, occurs in both females and males and, occurs regardless of how neurogenesis levels are increased (i.e. genetic, chemical, physical methods). Conversely, decreased neurogenesis reduces forgetting of previously acquired memories and slows the acquisition of new conflicting memories10. Together, these results demonstrate the importance of postnatal neurogenesis for maintaining the balance of stability versus plasticity and how this may preferentially modulate learning versus memory.

Neurogenesis induced forgetting is a robust and reliable phenomenon8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15. However, the mechanisms that mediate the reduction in memory strength have not been clearly identified. New neurons integrate into the existing hippocampal circuitry and as result may overwrite existing mature synapses16,17. As such, it has been proposed that neurogenesis causes forgetting via reconfiguration of the mossy fiber to CA3 circuitry, as newly born DG cells form connections, leading to a failure to reactivate the original memory trace8. Computational modelling of the effects of neurogenesis on hippocampal circuitry has found that the addition of new, more excitable units to the DG after the network has been trained on a given pattern causes a reduction in the rate of reactivation of CA3 units that were previously activated by that pattern18. Indeed, ablation of neurogenesis impairs the reactivation of engram cells in the CA3 during fear recall19, showing that newly born DG cells have a significant effect on the activity of the CA3 during memory retrieval.

Computational models and predictions about potential mechanisms have, to date, been heavily focused on the DG and its important interactions with immediate input and output structures (e.g. entorhinal cortex and CA320,21,22). Early in their maturation, postnatally-born granule cells preferentially synapse onto CA3 interneurons rather than excitatory neurons23 meaning that the addition of new neurons may not only have the effect of "overwriting" older mossy fiber connections, but also of causing a broad increase in inhibitory drive within the CA3. Although effects in the CA3 almost certainly play a role, neurogenesis has been shown to have downstream effects beyond the mossy fiber pathway. For example, increased neurogenesis causes increased inhibition in the CA3 and CA116,21. Given that the Schaffer collateral pathway has denser connectivity than the mossy fiber pathway24,25,26,27, neurogenesis-induced changes in CA3 activity could lead to even larger changes in CA1 activity. Thus, we sought here to examine the impact of postnatal neurogenesis on activity of the DG, CA3 and CA1.

We hypothesized that postnatal neurogenesis impairs the retrieval of previously acquired memories by altering the activity or excitability of downstream hippocampal subregions. We tested this hypothesis by examining neurogenesis-dependent changes in the activity of hippocampal subregions during memory retrieval. Our results point to altered CA1 activity that may be mediated by the deterioration of perineuronal nets as a mechanism underlying neurogenesis induced forgetting. These findings provide a mechanistic account of the retrograde effects of postnatal neurogenesis and also help to reconcile the pro-mnemonic anterograde effects of postnatal neurogenesis with the pro-forgetting effects observed in the retrograde direction.

Our first goal was to confirm the impact of voluntary exercise on memory retention in the retrograde direction (Fig. 1a). To do so, we first trained mice to establish contextual fear memory (Fig. 1b). To increase neurogenesis, half of the mice were given a running wheel for 4 weeks and the other half remained sedentary in standard housing conditions. Following this manipulation, we observed a significant decrease in memory retrieval in the running group (Fig. 1c) consistent with previous reports that elevated neurogenesis induces forgetting. To confirm that running increased neurogenesis, we examined the immature neuron marker DCX and observed a significant increase in the number of labeled cells in the dentate gyrus of runners (Fig. 1d,e). To determine whether the disruption of contextual memory following voluntary exercise is specific to memories acquired prior to the manipulation, we performed an anterograde style experiment where mice were given access to a running wheel prior to contextual fear conditioning (Supplementary Fig. S2a). We then tested the mice 30 days later. Despite the prior period of voluntary exercise, we observed no significant differences between control and running groups in contextual fear memory acquisition (Supplementary Fig. S2b) or subsequent retrieval (Supplementary Fig. S2c). Together these results demonstrate that voluntary exercise induces a large increase in neurogenesis and strongly modulates the strength of existing hippocampus-dependent memories.

Running induces neurogenesis, promotes forgetting of previously acquired information and alters CA1 activity. (a) After training in a contextual conditioning paradigm, mice were housed conventionally (n = 14) or given running wheel access (n = 14) for 30 days before testing memory retention. (b) There was no difference in the proportion of time spent freezing between groups during contextual fear conditioning. (c) Running promoted forgetting of the conditioned context as measured by a decrease in freezing compared to sedentary control mice. (d) Example of doublecortin labeling (cyan) in the dentate gyrus of mice from control and running groups. Scale bars, 50 μm. (e) Mice given access to running wheels showed an increase in neurogenesis. (f) Examples of c-fos + neurons (cyan) in hippocampus subregions during contextual fear conditioning memory retrieval. Hippocampus overview scale bar, 500 μm. High magnification scale bar, 50 μm. (g) Running did not alter the density of c-fos + neurons in the DG or CA3 but increased c-fos expression in area CA1. (h) This alteration is further illustrated in a plot of the effect size (Cohen's d), showing that there was only a condition-dependent decrease exceeding the bootstrapped 95% CI in CA1. Data analysis used Two-Sample T-Test (b,c,e), ANOVA (e) with Tukey's post-hoc test, and Multiple Two-Groups estimation statistics with Cohen's d as a measure of effect size (h). *P < 0.05. Data shown are mean ± s.e.m. See Supplementary Table S1 for full statistical analysis.

Several previous studies8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15 have demonstrated that the forgetting of previously acquired memories is induced by elevating the rate of postnatal neurogenesis and our current results confirm these previous findings. Our next goal was to determine whether we could observe disrupted patterns of hippocampal activity that underlie the neurogenesis induced forgetting as a first step towards determining the mechanism by which memories are weakened. To do so, we examined c-fos expression, as a proxy of activated neurons28,29,30,31,32, in different hippocampal subregions. We observed a significant group by region interaction in c-fos expression (Fig. 1f–g). There were no significant differences in the density of c-fos expression in the dentate gyus or area CA3. However, the running group had a significant decrease in c-Fos in area CA1 compared to the control group.

While the difference was significant only in CA1, we did observe a trend towards a decrease in c-fos expression in CA3 as well. To further analyze these changes, we performed estimation statistics to examine the effect sizes in each subregion. Interestingly, there appears to be a gradient of effect that increases from the DG, to CA3 and CA1 (Fig. 1f–g) suggesting that the impact of postnatal neurogenesis is smaller at the site of its physical integration than it is downstream in the hippocampal circuitry.

We also examined c-Fos expression from mice in anterograde condition where neurogenesis was modulated by voluntary exercise prior to memory acquisition. As discussed above there was no memory impairment in the anterograde condition as a result of running. Additionally, in this condition there was no effect of running on c-Fos expression in any of the hippocampal subregions during subsequent memory retrieval (Supplementary Fig. S2d,e). This strongly supports the notion that the reduction in CA1 c-Fos in the retrograde condition is a consequence of the decreased memory retention.

Having identified an activity dependent signature of impaired memory retrieval in area CA1, we next performed fiber photometry in this region to develop a more nuanced understanding of the activity changes related to neurogenesis induced forgetting. The same behavioural paradigm and timeline was used as above except that we virally expressed GCaMP7f33 to monitor the population activity of area CA1 during contextual fear learning and post running memory retrieval (Fig. 2a,b). As shown in our c-Fos experiments (Fig. 1), we demonstrate first that voluntary running increased neurogenesis (Fig. 2c,d) and, to the extent that neurogenesis was increased by voluntary running, we observed a significant decrease in freezing (Fig. 2e). We also found a significant correlation between neurogenesis and memory retention in runners (Fig. 2f). Prior to the voluntary exercise manipulation, we did not observe any group differences in contextual fear acquisition or in any fiber photometry related metrics during training (Supplementary Fig. S3). Following one month of running, we performed photometry recordings in a clean mouse cage to determine whether running had a non-specific (i.e., non mnemonic) effect on CA1 population activity (representative recordings presented in Supplementary Fig. S4). There was no significant difference between runner and sedentary groups with respect to baseline calcium activity (Fig. 2g) indicating that voluntary exercise does not cause a general shift in CA1 activity. We then immediately transferred the mice to the contextual fear chambers. Upon doing so we observed an increase in GCaMP7f. activity in the control group that was significantly attenuated in the runners (Fig. 2h). We calculated the area under the curve for each photometry trace and observed that this metric was significantly correlated with memory strength as measured by time spent freezing in both control and running groups (Fig. 2i,j). These initial photometry results are indicative of reduced population activity in CA1 following voluntary exercise that is specific to memory retrieval. This is consistent with the c-Fos expression changes shown in Fig. 1.

Running-induced neurogenesis increases behaviour-specific activity of CA1. (a) Mice were infected with GCaMP7f. prior to contextual fear conditioning. Mice were then housed conventionally (n = 14) or with a running wheel (n = 10) for 30 days prior to a memory test. Fiber Photometry was performed during training and memory retrieval. (b) Representative photomicrograph of GCaMP7f. expression (cyan). Overview scale bar, 100 μm. CA1 scale bar, 100 μm. (c) Runners showed an increase in neurogenesis. (d) Example of doublecortin labeling (cyan) in control and runners. Dentate overview scale bar, 100 μm. High magnification scale bar, 50 μm. (e) Running promoted forgetting of the context memory. (f) In runners, the increase in doublecortin labeling correlated with memory strength. (g) During photometry recordings in a clean home cage prior to the retention test, there was no difference in mean GCaMP7f. activity between runners and controls. (h) Once transferred to the conditioning chamber, control mice showed a significantly greater increase in mean GCaMP7f. activity from one context to the next compared to runners. (i) Mean GCaMP7f. fluorescence across the context memory test with the median group freezing plotted above. See figure—Supplemental Fig. 1 for non-baseline-corrected group mean traces and representative individual recordings. (j) In both groups the area under the GCaMP7f. fluorescence curve across the trial correlated significantly with percent freezing. Photometry recordings were then separated by behavioural expression. (k) Area under the curve was suppressed in runners irrespective of behavioural expression. (l) The mean peak height of the photometry signal did not differ across conditions or by behavioural expression. (m) Control mice showed an increase in GCaMP7f. peak frequency (peaks/s) during movement relative to freezing. Runners failed to show this behaviour-specific increase in frequency during movement. Data analysis used Two-Sample T-Test (c,e,g,h) and Two-way ANOVA (k,l,m) with Tukey's post-hoc test. *P < 0.05. Data shown are mean ± s.e.m. See Supplementary Table S2 for full statistical analysis.

We were next interested in determining whether we could differentiate control versus running group based on differential CA1 activity during freezing versus non-freezing behaviours. We examined AUC and observed a significant decrease in runners (Fig. 2k). However, this was consistently decreased during both movement and freezing. We also analyzed the peak frequency and while there was no significant main effect of group, there was a significant group by behaviour interaction. We examined the mean peak height and found that this metric was not significantly different between groups and also did not change as a function of freezing versus non-freezing behaviour (Fig. 2l). During bouts of freezing, both controls and runners showed low peak frequency. During bouts of movement, control mice exhibited a significant increase in CA1 peak frequency that was not observed in the running group (Fig. 2m). This suggests that the behaviour dependent differences in peak frequency are most likely not due to the recruitment of additional neurons (in which case an increase in peak height would be expected) but instead due to changes in the frequency of the same population of neurons34.

Perineuronal nets (PNNs) have proposed roles in modulating plasticity, excitability and memory strength35,36,37. It has previously been demonstrated that factors such as running wheel access and age alter the expression density of PNNs in the hippocampus38,39. While both factors also affect neurogenesis, a link between the two has only been hypothesized40. We speculated that the relationship between postnatal neurogenesis and the expression pattern of PNNs could potentially explain the associated decrease in memory expression. We therefore quantified the density of PNNs in the DG, CA3 and CA1 of mice that had undergone post-learning manipulation of neurogenesis with voluntary exercise (Fig. 3a). Intriguingly, we observed the same pattern of changes as was observed with c-Fos expression, that is, a running by region interaction with a reduction of PNN density in the CA1 of the running group (Fig. 3b,c). Using high magnification confocal microscopy, we examined the surface area of the remaining PNNs in the CA1 and found that, in addition to the decrease in overall number, there was also a decrease in the contiguity of the surface structure in the running group, potentially indicative of degradation of the PNNs (Fig. 3d–f). Interestingly, to examine the extent that perineuronal net expression is disrupted by neurogenesis per se, we correlated both the contiguity and density of PNNs with the number of DCX-labeled immature neurons. In both cases, there were strong inverse correlations in the control group (significant in the case of contiguity and a near significant trend with PNN density) whereas these correlations were weak and non-significant in the running group (Fig. 3g,h).

Running-induced neurogenesis decreases the density and contiguity of perineuronal nets in CA1. (a) Representative photomicrograph of PNN expression (cyan) across the hippocampus, with high magnification images of CA1, CA3, and the dentate gyrus. Hippocampus overview scale bar, 500 μm. High magnification scale bar, 50 μm. (b) PNN expression density in controls (n = 8) and runners (n = 7). Scale bars, 250 μm. (c) Running-induced neurogenesis decreased the expression density of PNNs in CA1. (d) High-resolution images of PNNs were collected from the CA1 and (e) binarized using thresholding to assess contiguity. Scale bars, 25 μm. (f) Running-induced neurogenesis decreased the contiguity of PNNs in CA1. In the control condition, the density of doublecortin + cells trended highly in the anti-correlated direction with (g) CA1 PNN expression density and (h) CA1 PNN contiguity, while neither of these correlations were significant in the running condition. Data analysis used ANOVA (c) with Tukey's test during post-hoc multiple comparisons and Two-Sample T-Test (f). *P < 0.05. Data shown are mean ± s.e.m. See Supplementary Table S3 for full statistical analysis.

To further determine whether neurogenesis is indeed the factor that is modulating the expression of perineuronal nets in CA1, we investigated additional regulators of postnatal neurogenesis. As an additional mechanism of increasing neurogenesis, we treated mice with memantine11 and to decrease neurogenesis we used temozolomide41 (TMZ). Compared to saline injected mice, memantine treatment resulted in a significant decrease in PNN expression in CA1 (but not DG or CA3), just as was observed with voluntary running. TMZ, on the other hand, reduced neurogenesis and subsequently increase the expression of PNNs in CA1 (Fig. 4a,b).

The activity of immature granule cells in the dentate gyrus is inversely related to the density of perineuronal nets in CA1. (a) 4 weeks of TMZ treatment (n = 5) increased the expression density and memantine treatment (n = 5) decreased the expression density of PNNs in CA1 relative to saline-treated controls (n = 5). This treatment effect was not present in the dentate gyrus or CA3. (b) Memantine treatment induced an increase in neurogenesis, while TMZ treatment reduced neurogenesis relative to saline-treated controls. (c) The expression density of PNNs in CA1 increased with age throughout adolescence, during which time (d) neurogenesis decreased. (e) Optic fibers were implanted in the dentate gyrus of tg + (n = 4) and tg- (n = 4) Nestin-ChR2 mice 14 days prior to contextual conditioning. Nestin-expressing immature neurons in the dentate gyrus were then stimulated for 5 min daily at 10 Hz for 14 days. Mice were reintroduced into the conditioned context 28 days after conditioning. (f) During the reintroduction to the conditioned context, ChR2 tg + mice showed impaired memory relative to ChR2 tg- controls. (g) ChR2 tg + mice also showed a significant decrease in CA1 PNN expression density. (h) These changes in memory retrieval and CA1 PNN expression density occurred in the absence of differences in the expression density of doublecortin + immature neurons. Data analysis used ANOVA (a–d) with Tukey's test during post-hoc multiple comparisons and Two-Sample T-Test (f–h). *P < 0.05. Data shown are mean ± s.e.m. See Supplementary Table S4 for full statistical analysis.

Post-natal neurogenesis in the hippocampus is strongly dependent on the age of the animal. Juvenile mice have significantly greater levels of neurogenesis than do aged mice, and the decrease in neurogenesis begins early in life14,42. Therefore, we sought to use age as an additional natural modulator of postnatal neurogenesis to examine differences in PNN expression. If the integration of postnatally generated neurons does in fact negatively regulate CA1 PNN expression, then we would expect an increase in PNN expression with increasing age. To test this, we quantified PNN expression in CA1 of mice that were 1, 2 or 4 months old. As expected, we observed a significant age-dependent decrease in neurogenesis and a corresponding age-dependent increase in PNNs in CA1 (Fig. 4c,d)43. These results provide further supporting evidence that the levels of postnatal neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus impact CA1 PNN expression.

Together, the results of these various manipulations strongly suggest that postnatal neurogenesis modulated PNN expression in area CA1. But how does postnatal neurogenesis in the DG impact PNN expression in CA1? Direct and/or local neuronal activity has previously been linked to modification of PNNs44,45,46. We hypothesized that the activity of postnatal-generated neurons could alter the circuit activity of the DG resulting in PNN changes in CA1. To examine the impact of new neuron activity, we used a Nestin-Cre/ERT2 mouse line to express ChR2 in immature neurons. Optogenetic stimulation of this immature neuron population was applied daily for 2 weeks beginning 24 h after contextual fear conditioning. This optogenetic approach was sufficient to induce forgetting of the previously acquired contextual fear memory when tested one month after initial learning (Fig. 4e,f). The optogenetic stimulation significantly decreased PNN expression in CA1 compared to the non-stimulated control group (Fig. 4g). However, it did not increase the number of DCX + cells in the DG (Fig. 4h). Together these findings demonstrate that the activity of immature neurons in the DG can have a direct impact on CA1 PNN expression.

We and others have demonstrated that elevated postnatal neurogenesis is inversely related to the strength of memories acquired before neurogenesis is increased8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15. Here, we sought to examine the mechanisms underlying this form of neurogenesis-induced forgetting (Fig. 5). We began by identifying changes in experience-dependent c-Fos expression in the HPC associated with forgetting. We found that reduced contextual fear memory retrieval was accompanied by reduced c-Fos expression specifically within the CA1 subfield. This result points to the importance of CA1 in memory retrieval and suggests that postnatal neurogenesis disrupts memory storage and/or retrieval in this region. The CA1 has been shown to be active during recall of context fear memories47,48 and impairments in fear memory recall can be induced though optogenetic disruption of the CA149,50. The CA1 has also been proposed by some to act not as a storage region per se but as a decoding circuit for memories stored in an orthogonalized fashion in CA351. Regardless, of the exact function of the CA1, disruption of its activity clearly has critical implications for memory retrieval.

Summary of findings. Combined, these results show that increased neurogenesis causes large downstream effects in the activity and PNN expression density of area CA1. Interestingly, the effects are absent in the DG and CA3, where no differences in PNN of c-Fos expression densities were reported, despite these areas being more proximal to postnatally-born granule cells. On this basis, we suggest that important mechanisms of neurogenesis-induced forgetting lie in physiological changes appearing distally and downstream in the CA1.

To further investigate the activity changes in CA1 associated with neurogenesis-induced forgetting, we performed in vivo fiber photometry. If increased neurogenesis (or alternatively non-specific effects of voluntary exercise) resulted in a generalized decrease in activity that blocked retrieval, then we would predict a decreased baseline Ca2+ signal in Ca1. However, we did not observe a baseline difference in activity but instead demonstrated that forgetting was associated with an overall context-specific reduction in CA1 population CA2+ signal as well as a behaviourally dependent decrease in peak frequency during movement. The decrease of these two proxies of neuronal activity in the CA1 during impaired memory retrieval suggests that the CA1 plays a critical role in the storage of context memories and that increased neurogenesis induces forgetting by modulating memory storage in CA1.

There are important controls which should be considered when assessing fiber photometry data, to prevent the influence of motion artifacts on the signal. In addition to the 470-nm GCaMP channel, we also recorded fluorescence intensity from a 415-nm channel as is standard for fiber photometry52,53,54,55,56. The 415-nm channel is consistent with the isosbestic point of GCaMP7f33; therefore the resulting fluorescence intensity recorded in response to this wavelength is considered to be independent of calcium binding. Deviations in fluorescence signal observed in the isosbestic channel are then presumed to be movement and noise-related events and are controlled for post hoc in the calcium-dependent GCaMP signal by subtracting the noise-related signal after having linearly scaled the signals to account for channel-related differences in response amplitude57,58.

In the current study, we synchronized the photometry signal with behavioural recordings. This allowed for us to compare photometry signals during discrete bouts of movement and while mice were freezing, a proxy of memory retention. During contextual memory recall, the areas under the curve of the photometry traces were highly correlated with movement (Fig. 2j) but unlikely to be due to a motion artifact. If this correlation were the result of motion artifact, it would have also been expected during the conditioning session. However, we found that the magnitudes of the areas under the curve of the photometry traces do not correlate with movement during acquisition of contextual memory (Supplemental Fig. S3c). This suggests that the relationship between photometry signal and movement is not related to an artifact of motion but instead relates to the species-specific freezing response as a proxy of contextual fear memory. A potential caveat to the photometry analyses conducted in the current project is a lack of locomotor tracking during the baseline recording period; however, there were no differences in locomotor behaviour observed during context conditioning (Supplemental Fig. S3a).

After assessing the effects of neurogenesis on the activity of CA1, we next investigated neurogenesis-induced changes in perineuronal nets in hippocampal subregions based on the proposed functions of these extracellular matrix components in modulating, plasticity and excitability59. We provide congruent evidence of the impact of positive modulators (running9,10,12, memantine11) and negative modulators (TMZ9, aging60) of postnatal neurogenesis on PNN expression in area CA1 of the hippocampus. While decreasing neurogenesis was associated with increased PNN expression within area CA1, increasing neurogenesis was associated with decreased PNN expression in CA1.

Prior evidence indicates that perineuronal nets in area CA1 protect the stability of long-term memories and, that their removal decreases memory storage through a mechanism involving increased activity of Parvalbumin expressing interneurons35. It has also been suggested that elimination of CA1 perineuronal nets acts to restrict long-term depression (LTD)37,61. Given that LTD can lead to the elimination of synapses62 this too could lead to a decrease in memory expression. Either, or both of these mechanisms could be induced by the reduction in CA1 perineuronal net expression that we identified from increasing the number of, or activity of postnatally generated neurons. These mechanisms are also consistent with decreased patterns of CA1 activity that we have observed here. If perineuronal net reduction increases the frequency of PV interneurons as has been previously shown, then a resulting decrease in the excitation of CA1 would be expected as was observed in terms of c-Fos expression and GCAMP7 activity.

Our findings of changes in CA1 PNN expression density with aging and voluntary exercise are consistent with previous reports38,39. However, these previous reports did not assess neurogenesis, a process which is altered under both conditions. In the present study, we demonstrate altered PNN expression density in the CA1 across five conditions in which postnatal born neurons are altered. Additionally, we show here that direct optogenetic stimulation of immature postnatally generated neurons in the dentate gyrus, without alteration to the number of new neurons, both induced forgetting of a previously acquired memory and induces remodelling of the perineuronal nets in area CA1. This pattern replicates the behavioural phenomenon that we have observed with running as the modulator of postnatal neurogenesis. Previously it has been demonstrated using combined running and transgenic inhibition of postnatal neurogenesis, that the effects of running on forgetting of previously acquired memories are specifically dependent on the increase in neurogenesis. Our observation, that direct optogenetic stimulation of new neurons induces forgetting, provides further evidence of the specific effect of increased excitability of postnatally generated neurons associated with neurogenesis in this phenomenon.

Recent work has demonstrated that the density of perineuronal nets in the hippocampus can be bidirectionally modified by direct artificial stimulation or inhibition using DREADDs46. However, to the best of our knowledge our current results are among the first to demonstrate activity induced disruption of perineuronal nets occurring via a polysynaptic pathway rather than stimulation that is either localized to the region of interest or targeted to the PNNs themselves. This further emphasizes that although postnatal neurogenesis may modulate the structural connectivity of the local circuitry in the dentate gyrus, and proximally with CA3, the functional consequences of new neuron activity may be observed in areas that are not directly connected to the dentate gyrus. Previous research has found that new neurons in the DG preferentially incorporate with hilar interneurons and CA3 interneurons23. Thus, increased neurogenesis could have resulted in a shift toward more inhibitory drive from the DG onto the CA3, resulting in decreased c-Fos expression downstream of the DG. In addition, it has been shown that increased neurogenesis causes increased inhibition in the CA3 and CA1, with increased activity of parvalbumin interneurons in both regions and a decrease in CA1 sharp wave ripples63.

An explanation for why our c-fos and PNN effects appears largest in the CA1 may be that DG-CA3 connections are rather sparse compared with CA3-CA1 connections24,25,27, meaning in theory that a small change in population activity in the CA3 could have amplified downstream effects in the CA1.

The present study provides evidence of neurogenesis-induced forgetting in female mice and suggests that this mechanism may be mediated in part by disrupting perineuronal nets in CA1 which may otherwise protect memories from degradation. This behavioural effect of neurogenesis induced forgetting has previously been demonstrated in both female and male rodents9,10,11,13 although it is possible that the underlying mechanisms may differ. Sex differences exist both in behaviour during contextual conditioning64,65,66 and in rates of neurogenesis67,68,69, therefore, future studies could extend upon these results and assess whether these changes in CA1 PNN expression density are also present in male mice.

The results of the present study are consistent with theories of neurogenesis reducing the impact of proactive interference8,9,70. Due to the much greater rates of neurogenesis in young brains, it has been proposed that postnatal neurogenesis may be of primary importance to juveniles as a mechanism of circuit sculpting rather than as a component of ongoing hippocampal function71,72. However, postnatal neurogenesis does continue in adult and aged animals, albeit at a lower rate73,74,75,76,77,78. Computational modeling has demonstrated that even rates of neurogenesis producing as few as 0.2% of the total population of DG granule cells are sufficient to induce behavioural effects consistent with reduced proactive interference79. As such, we do not believe these two theories to be incompatible or mutually exclusive.

In the present study, we can only report the effects observed within the context of the mouse brain and as such, it remains unknown whether neurogenesis in humans could play a similar role in forgetting and regulation of proactive interference. Neurogenesis has been documented in the adult human brain by numerous studies78,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87. Several recent studies have reignited a controversy over the existence of at least the magnitude of postnatal neurogenesis in the adult brain88,89,90. In the many studies that have observed neurogenesis in the adult brain the rate of new neuron production is relatively low78. However, as discussed above, computational modelling has suggested that even very low levels of postnatal neurogenesis may induce forgetting and as such, the low levels of neurogenesis in adult humans does not preclude this function in the human brain. Based on the existing state of knowledge it is not yet possible to determine the translatability of neurogenesis induced forgetting.

Optimization of both learning and memory requires a balance between circuit plasticity and stability. Previous work has demonstrated that neurogenesis induced forgetting is accompanied by facilitation of new learning10. This multifaceted effect could be explained simply by an increase in plasticity which would shift the balance of the circuit to promote acquisition and disrupt memories that require the stability of that circuit for retrieval. Mechanistically, we provide strong evidence that postnatal neurogenesis alters this circuit balance through an activity dependent modulation of perineuronal nets in area CA1. In the current studies we investigated mechanisms that relate to neurogenesis induced forgetting but, this is not the only function that neurogenesis has been implicated in. Postnatal neurogenesis has been shown or theorized to play numerous roles including mood regulation91,92,93,94,95, cognitive flexibility96,97,98, pattern separation4,12,99,100, long-term memory101,102,103. As such, we do not expect there to be a singular function of postnatal neurogenesis. The interaction between postnatally generated neurons and the existing neuronal circuitry is complex and there may be separate downstream mechanism that influence these other factors (e.g. cognition or mood). However, there may also be a common mechanism with respect to the modulation of PNNs that could mediate, in part, several of the proposed functions of postnatal neurogenesis. For example, PNN expression in multiple brain regions including CA1 is implicated in stress and mood disorders104,105,106. Similarly, PNNs are important for learning61,107, memory35,59,108, cognitive flexibility109,110 and developmental circuit sculpting111,112. To determine the extent to which altered PNN expression mediates other functions of postnatal neurogenesis will require additional future work.

We used two strains of mice. C57Bl/6N background mice (JAX) were used for most experiments. A transgenic mouse line expressing CreERT2 under the nestin promoter sequence (JAX) was also used for optogenetic targeting of immature neurons. Female mice were used in all experiments and were 7 weeks old at the start of testing. Mice were housed in standard cages with three to five mice per cage and free access to food and water. The room lighting was maintained on a 12 h/12 h light/dark cycle (8 am, lights on). All behavioural testing and photometry experiments were conducted during the light cycle phase. Experiments were conducted in accordance with the policies and guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care and were approved by the University of Calgary animal care committee. The study is reported in accordance with ARRIVE guidelines.

To increase neurogenesis, mice were given voluntary access to a running wheel (Fast Trac™, Med Associates Inc., Fairfax, VT, United States, 16 cm in diameter) in their home cage for one month. Voluntary running is a highly reproducible and widely used approach to increase neurogenesis. It was chosen here because it has been used in previous experiments examining neurogenesis-induced forgetting where the behavioural effects were found to be specifically dependent on the increase in neurogenesis9,10,12. Mice were trained to use running wheels by gently placing them on the running wheel and obstructing their path off of the wheel. Mice were considered trained if they ran on the wheel for approximately 30 s. Wheel revolutions were recorded continuously using the Wheel Manager software program (Med Associates Inc., Fairfax, VT, United States) and the number of wheel rotations was divided by the number of mice in the cage to give an estimated running distance per mouse. Mice ran on average 8.1 km (± s.e.m. 1.1 km) per day. Mice in the sedentary group were housed conventionally with no running wheel but had standard enrichment items (dome house and nesting material).

To quantify the effects of neurogenesis on perineuronal nets groups of mice were also treated with temozolomide (TMZ) to decrease neurogenesis or Memantine to increase neurogenesis. Control mice were injected with 0.9% saline. TMZ was administered as previously described41. We injected mice on 3 consecutive days per week for 4 weeks with a dose of 25 mg/kg (i.p.). Memantine was injected once per week for 4 weeks at a dose of 25 mg/kg (i.p.). Finally, we also used age as a naturalistic modulator of postnatal neurogenesis. C57bl/6 N female mice were perfused at either 4, 8 or 12 weeks of age and perineuronal nets were labeled as described below.

Prior to contextual fear conditioning, all mice were handled for 5 min each day for 3 to 5 days until mice were calm with the handler. The Ugo Basile (Gemonio, Italy) contextual fear conditioning chambers (17 cm × 17 cm × 24.7 cm) were placed in noise reducing cabinets. The conditioning environment had shock grid floors (bars spaced 0.5 cm apart, 0.2 cm in diameter). Behaviour was monitored using overhead infrared cameras and scored automatically using automated tracking software (ANY-Maze, Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL, United States). During the 5-min training trial, mice were allowed to explore the chamber for two minutes before being delivered three shocks (1 mA, 2 s) each separated by an interval of 1 min. Mice were removed from the conditioning chamber one minute after the last shock and placed back in their home cage undisturbed for 24 h. Then, running wheels were introduced to the half of the mice. After four weeks of manipulating neurogenesis, mice were tested in the contextual fear task for five minutes with no shocks. The running wheel was locked the day prior to behavioural testing to prevent a direct effect of exercise on activity levels during testing. Freezing was used as the primary measure of memory retention and was defined as a complete lack of motion, except for respiration, for at least one second. Freezing was automatically calculated by the ANY-maze software system and was spot-checked for accuracy by an experimenter blind to treatment conditions. All mice were perfused 90 min following the context memory test in order to quantify c-Fos expression throughout the hippocampus.

In a separate experiment, mice were first given access to a running wheel for one month or housed without a wheel under standard conditions. The running wheels were then removed and the mice were trained on a contextual fear conditioning paradigm as described above. The mice were then tested one month later for context memory. All mice were perfused 90 min following the context memory test in order to quantify c-Fos expression throughout the hippocampus.

Surgeries were conducted under isoflurane anaesthesia delivered via a Somnosuite anaesthetic delivery system (Kent Scientific) drawing pure compressed oxygen. Mice were induced at 5% isoflurane concentration before being transferred to a stereotaxic head frame (Kopf) and maintained at ~ 2% isoflurane. Internal body temperature was monitored with a rectal thermometer and regulated with a homeothermic pad. Analgesia (Anafen; 5 mg/kg; s.c) and fluid support (Lactated Ringer's solution; s.c) were given at the beginning of the surgery. The scalp was shaved and cleaned with alternating chlorhexidine and 70% ethanol scrubs and then incised along the midline and the skull scrubbed with 3% H2O2. Using a robotic stereotaxic manipulator paired with Stereodrive software (Neurostar), the locations of bregma and lambda were measured as well as two points 2 mm to either side of the midline to allow the stereotaxic software to correct for any slight imperfections in skull flatness. After obtaining the coordinates of the injection and implant site, a burr hole was drilled in the skull (AP -2.18; ML ± 2.1; DV to brain surface). A glass infusion needle attached to a Nanoject III infusion system (Drummond Scientific) and backfilled with mineral oil was slowly lowered into the CA1 (AP − 2.18; ML ± 2.0; DV 1.4) before injecting 250 nL of virus (pGP-AAV1-syn-jGCaMP7f.-SV40-WPRE; Addgene) in 50 nL pulses each interspersed by ~ 10 s and the final pulse followed by a 10 min period where the needle was left in place to allow for diffusion. At the end of the 10 min, the needle was gradually raised out of the brain before a fiber optic implant (N.A. 0.37, core 200 µm 2 mm length; Neurophotometrics) was lowered into the CA1 just lateral to the injection site (AP − 2.18; ML ± 2.1; DV 1.4). Implants were secured to the skull by applying a thin layer of Metabond to the exposed surface of the skull and around the base of the implant. This layer was then covered with black opaque dental acrylic to form a headcap which was given time to harden before the incision was closed with suture material. Mice were then removed from the stereotaxic frame and allowed to recover for 2 weeks while receiving additional Anafen doses for 3 days after surgery.

Prior to behavioural training and in vivo fiber photometry recordings, mice were habituated to being connected to the fiber optic patch cord for 3 consecutive days. Fiber photometry was conducted using a FP3002 Neurophotometrics system controlled by Bonsai software113. The Neurophotometrics system also received TTL pulses generated by Anymaze software (Stoelting), which controlled the fear conditioning chambers, in order to synchronize Ca2+ recordings with the behaviour. Excitation light was delivered at 470 nm, while a 415 nm channel was used as an isosbestic control. Recordings were acquired at 40 FPS with the 2 channels being active in alternating frames, resulting in an effective framerate of 20/s for each channel. Light power was calibrated using a photometer (Thor Labs PM100D) so that each channel emitted at approximately 50 μW. During behaviour testing, the fiber patch cord (Doric) was secured to the mouse's fiber implant with a ceramic collar and the mouse was left undisturbed with no recording for 30 s after the attachment of the patch cord. Following this, a 5 min baseline recording was acquired while the mouse sat alone in a clean transfer cage with bedding. Locomotor activity was not recorded during this baseline recording. At the 5 min mark, mice were transferred to the fear conditioning apparatus for training or testing.

To synchronize photometry recordings with behavioural data, a TTL pulse was sent to the Neurophotometrics system upon the start of the ANY-maze protocol. Photometry recordings were analyzed using custom MATLAB scripts. Behavioural information recorded using ANY-maze was integrated into the analysis by indexing through the photometry data and aligning behavioural data based on the minimum difference between pairs of timestamps. Bouts of movement or immobility which were shorter than the freezing criteria of 2 s were not included in the analysis. Data from the isosbestic 415 nm channel was fit to a biexponential decay to correct for photobleaching114. The resulting vector was used to linearly scale calcium-dependent data collected using a 470 nm LED114. To calculate ΔF, these linearly scaled calcium-dependent data were subtracted from the raw unprocessed calcium-dependent data. The resulting values were divided by the linearly scaled calcium-dependent data to generate a ΔF/F trace.

For analyses of ΔF/F, traces were baseline corrected (Eq. 1). This was done for each mouse by calculating the mean ΔF/F value during the middle 3 min of the 5-min home cage recording. This period was chosen as the baseline period to reduce the likelihood that handling stress would influence the resulting value. The difference between the mean baseline ΔF/F and the minimum ΔF/F value recorded during the test session was then added to each value during the test. This correction was applied to account for traces in which the ΔF/F dropped below the X-axis during the test. The resulting ΔF/F traces were analyzed for area under the curve (AUC), peak frequency, and mean peak height. AUC was operationally defined as the summed area between the X-axis and the ΔF/F trace. Comparisons of AUC during specific behaviours were normalized by the summed duration of the specific behavioural epochs. Analyses of peak frequency and mean peak height employed a peak detection filter of 2 standard deviations above the median ΔF/F value of the session being analyzed115.

where hc is a vector representing the ΔF/F values during the home cage baseline sampling period, test is a vector representing the ΔF/F values during the testing period, and n represents the number of samples in the test vector. Equation (1) represents the calculation of the corrected ΔF/F during the test session.

Nestin-cre mice underwent surgery using the surgical procedures similar to those described above. Here floxed channelrhodopsin expressing (AAV1-EF1a-double floxed-hChR2(H134R)-EYFP-WPRE-HGHpA) or GFP expressing (pAAV1-Ef1a-DIO EYFP) AAV viruses were infused bilaterally (0.25 µl) into the dentate gyrus (AP—1.94, ML ± 1.25, DV 1.8). Fiber optic probes were implanted as described above. The mice were then left for 2 weeks to allow for recovery and viral expression. Mice were then injected with tamoxifen, 180 mg/kg per day) for 3 consecutive days and were then left for an additional 2 weeks before beginning behavioural experiments. Mice were first trained in the contextual fear conditioning paradigm as described above. Then, 24 h following training mice received optogenetic stimulation with 0.4 mW, 10 Hz blue light for 1 min, followed by 3 min of no stimulation. This was repeated consecutively 5 times for a total of 5 min of stimulation over a period of 20 min. This stimulation protocol was repeated daily for 2 weeks. One month following behavioural training and 2 weeks after the completion of optogenetic stimulation the mice were tested for contextual fear memory and were then perfused.

Ninety minutes following the completion of the contextual conditioning task, mice were deeply anesthetized with isofluorane and then transcardially perfused with 0.1 M PBS and 4% formaldehyde. Brains were extracted and postfixed in 4% formaldehyde at 4 °C for 24 h. Brains were then cryoprotected with 30% sucrose in 0.1 M phosphate buffer at 4 °C for three to five days until they sank. Brains were sectioned 40 μm thick on a cryostat (Leica CM 1950, Concord, ON, Canada) in 12 series for the c-Fos experiment and in 6 series for the fiber photometry experiment (in order to ensure that fiber tracks could be reliably located). Sections were kept in a buffered antifreeze solution containing 30% ethylene glycol, and 20% glycerol in 0.1 M PBS and stored at − 20 °C.

Tissue sections were washed three times in 0.1 M PBS at room temperature for ten minutes each. Sections were then incubated for 48 h in a primary antibody solution containing either 1:500 goat anti-DCX antibody (c-18, sc-8066, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Dallas, Texas, United States), 4% normal donkey serum, 0.4% Triton X, and 0.1 M PBS for the c-Fos experiment or, for the fiber photometry and PNN experiments, 1:200 rabbit anti-DCX antibody (Cell Signalling, 4604 s), 3% normal goat serum, and 0.003% Triton-X in 0.1 M PBS. Sections then underwent three washes in 0.1 M PBS for ten minutes before being incubated for 24 h in a secondary antibody solution containing 1:500 Alexa Fluor 488 antibody (donkey anti-goat, CLAS10-1116, CedarLane Labs, Burlington, ON, Canada (or 1:500 Alexa Fluor 647 antibody goat anti-rabbit). Finally, sections were incubated with 1:5000 DAPI and 0.1 M PBS for 20 min and then washed twice in 0.1 M PBS for ten mins per wash. For each wash step, primary/secondary antibody solution, or DAPI incubation, the tissue was gently oscillated at room temperature. Sections were then mounted on glass slides and coverslipped with No. 1.5H coverslips using PVA-DABCO mounting medium.

DCX labelled cells were counted in the granular and SGZ of the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus using an Olympus BX63 epifluorescent microscope and 60X oil immersion objective. Cells were counted if the cell was circular or ovular and the cell body could be clearly distinguished. Cells with a granular cytoplasmic pattern of fluorescence were excluded. DG area was quantified based on the DAPI counterstain by tracing the granule cell layer in each section using FIJI.

Tissue sections were washed three times in 0.1 M PBS and then transferred to a primary antibody solution containing 1:500 rabbit anti-c-Fos antibody (226003, Synaptic Systems, Gottingen, Germany), 4% normal goat serum, 0.4% Triton X, and 0.1 M PBS for 48 h. Tissue sections were then washed in 0.1 M PBS three times for ten minutes before being transferred to a secondary antibody solution for 24 h. Secondary antibody solution contained 1:500 Alexa Fluor 488 antibody goat anti-rabbit (4412S, Cell Signaling Technologies, Danvers, MA, United States) with 0.1 M PBS. Finally, brain sections were incubated with 1:5000 DAPI and then washed twice in 0.1 M PBS. For each wash step, primary/secondary antibody solution, or DAPI incubation, the tissue was gently oscillated at room temperature. Sections were then mounted and coversliped with PVA-DABCO mounting medium.

c-Fos labeled tissue was imaged using an Olympus FV3000 confocal microscope. All acquisition settings were kept constant across images. Images were acquired with a 10X objective and 2X optical zoom (total magnification of 20X), with the aperture set to 1 Airy unit. Z-stacks step size was 3.64 μm. Images were separated into DAPI and c-Fos channels and then maximum intensity projected as a 2D image using FIJI.

To minimize variability and bias in our c-Fos cell counts we validated and used a machine learning based approach (Supplementary Fig. S1) photomicrographs of c-Fos + cells were segmented and converted to a binary image using the machine learning program Ilastik116 and quantified using FIJI. First, a training set of images which were representative of the experimental images were imported to Ilastik which was then trained to distinguish signal from background. This set of training images allowed Ilastik to classify cell from background based on pixel features including pixel intensity, edge features, and texture and object features such as object size, shape, and intensity. Following training, sample ROIs from 12 mice (five sedentary, seven running) from the DG, CA3 and CA1 region were uploaded to Ilastik to test the program. To compare the Ilastik-generated cell counts to ground truth values, the ROIs were also counted manually by four independent researchers who were experienced with quantifying c-Fos but were blind to the Ilastik-generated counts as well as to the counts of the other experimenters. The cell counts were summed for each region and then compared to Ilastik-generated counts. After training and validation, the experimental c-Fos images were batch processed. The binary images were imported to FIJI and overlaid with the DAPI image. Cells were then quantified using the binary image and area measured using FIJI. Area was then converted to mm2. Cells were quantified within the DG, CA3 and CA1 regions and expressed as a density (cells per mm2).

PNNs were labelled via Wisteria Floribunda Lectin (WFA) staining. Cryoprotected sections were rinsed 3 times in 0.1 M PBS and were then incubated in 0.2% triton-x in Carbo-free blocking buffer (VectorLABS) for 30 min. Then, the sections were stained in a 1:1000 dilution of FITC labeled WFA (VectorLABS) diluted in 1 × carbo-free blocking buffer containing 0.05% tween-20 for 24 h in the dark. Sections were then rinsed 3 times in PBS and counterstained with DAPI before being mounted and coverslipped with vectashield mounting medium (VectorLABS).

WFA labelled perineuronal nets were manually counted in the granular zones of the dentate gyrus, area CA1, and area CA3 of the hippocampus. Counts were conducted using an Olympus BX63 epifluorescent microscope and 60X oil immersion objective by an experimenter blind to treatment condition. The areas of the granule zones of each region were quantified based on DAPI counterstaining. Granule cell layers were traced in each region from every section using CellSens (Olympus). Cells were quantified within the DG, CA3 and CA1 regions and expressed as a density (cells per mm2).

Surface contiguity of CA1 perineuronal nets was assessed using an Olympus FV3000 confocal microscope. Image stacks of 5 randomly selected PNNs in the granular zone of area CA1 were collected from each mouse. Images were acquired with a 40X objective and 2.89X optical zoom (total magnification of 115.6X), with a numerical aperture of 0.95. Z-stacks step size was 0.37 μm. Imaging parameters were consistent across all scans. Image stacks were maximum intensity projected as 2D images using FIJI with the Z range set to encompass only the top half of each PNN. WFA signal was binarized in FIJI using default pixel intensity thresholding and individual PNNs were traced. Surface contiguity was defined as the fraction of WFA signal that resides within the largest connected component of the WFA signal after signal thresholding117.

Virus expression and fiber optic placement were verified by an experimenter blind to treatment condition and experimental outcomes. Virus expression in CA1 was present in all mice. Two control mice and three runners were removed from the photometry experiment because the tip of the optic fiber was positioned below CA1.

Statistical analysis was conducted using GraphPad Prism 8. Independent t-tests, two-way ANOVAs and Pearson Correlations were performed. Post-hoc Newman-Keuls post hoc tests were applied following ANOVA where appropriate. Grubbs’ analysis was utilized to identify any potential outliers in the Ilastik validation data. Hypothesis testing was complemented by estimation statistics for c-Fos quantification using estimationstats.com118. A multiple two-group analysis was used for c-Fos expression. For each two-group comparison, effect size (Cohen's d) is calculated using a bootstrap sampling distribution using 5000 resamples along with a 95.0% confidence interval (CI; bias-corrected and accelerated). The data are plotted using Cumming estimation plots for multiple two-group analysis which show individual data points and the effect size of the comparisons. Power analyses were performed based on behavioural data from prior studies of neurogenesis induced forgetting10. Sample sizes of 10–15 were estimated to give power of ~ 0.8–0.9. All statistical comparisons and outputs are included in Supplementary Tables S1-S6.

The data that support the findings of this study are available in the manuscript or supplementary materials, and available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

All analysis code used to generate the results reported in the manuscript, instructions on how to use these analyses, and sample datasets have been made publicly available in a GitHub repository (https://github.com/dterstege/PublicationRepo/tree/main/Evans2022).

Cushman, J. D., Drew, M. R. & Krasne, F. B. The environmental sculpting hypothesis of juvenile and adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Prog. Neurobiol. 199, 101961 (2021).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Van der Borght, K., Havekes, R., Bos, T., Eggen, B. J. L. & Van der Zee, E. A. Exercise improves memory acquisition and retrieval in the Y-maze task: Relationship with hippocampal neurogenesis. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 324–334 (2007).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Tronel, S. et al. Adult-born neurons are necessary for extended contextual discrimination. Hippocampus 22, 292–298 (2010).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Sahay, A. et al. Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation. Nature 472, 466–470 (2011).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Nilsson, M., Perfilieva, E., Johansson, U., Orwar, O. & Eriksson, P. S. Enriched environment increases neurogenesis in the adult rat dentate gyrus and improves spatial memory. J. Neurobiol. 39, 569–578 (1999).

3.0.CO;2-F" data-track-action="article reference" href="https://doi.org/10.1002%2F%28SICI%291097-4695%2819990615%2939%3A4%3C569%3A%3AAID-NEU10%3E3.0.CO%3B2-F" aria-label="Article reference 5" data-doi="10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695(19990615)39:43.0.CO;2-F">Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Marlatt, M. W., Potter, M. C., Lucassen, P. J. & van Praag, H. Running throughout middle-age improves memory function, hippocampal neurogenesis, and BDNF levels in female C57BL/6J mice. Dev. Neurobiol. 72, 943–952 (2012).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Stone, S. S. D. et al. Stimulation of entorhinal cortex promotes adult neurogenesis and facilitates spatial memory. J. Neurosci. 31, 13469–13484 (2011).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Frankland, P. W., Köhler, S. & Josselyn, S. A. Hippocampal neurogenesis and forgetting. Trends Neurosci. 36, 497–503 (2013).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Akers, K. G. et al. Hippocampal neurogenesis regulates forgetting during adulthood and infancy. Science 344, 598–602 (2014).

Article ADS CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Epp, J. R., Silva Mera, R., Köhler, S., Josselyn, S. A. & Frankland, P. W. Neurogenesis-mediated forgetting minimizes proactive interference. Nat. Commun. 7, 10838 (2016).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Ishikawa, R., Fukushima, H., Frankland, P. W. & Kida, S. Hippocampal neurogenesis enhancers promote forgetting of remote fear memory after hippocampal reactivation by retrieval. Elife 5, e17464 (2016).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Gao, A. et al. Elevation of hippocampal neurogenesis induces a temporally graded pattern of forgetting of contextual fear memories. J. Neurosci. 38, 3190–3198 (2018).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Cuartero, M. I. et al. Abolition of aberrant neurogenesis ameliorates cognitive impairment after stroke in mice. J. Clin. Invest. 129, 1536–1550 (2019).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Scott, G. A. et al. Adult neurogenesis mediates forgetting of multiple types of memory in the rat. Mol. Brain 14, 97 (2021).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Epp, J. R., Botly, L. C. P., Josselyn, S. A. & Frankland, P. W. Voluntary exercise increases neurogenesis and mediates forgetting of complex paired associates memories. Neuroscience 475, 1–9 (2021).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Toni, N. et al. Neurons born in the adult dentate gyrus form functional synapses with target cells. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 901–907 (2008).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Murray, K. D., Liu, X.-B., King, A. N., Luu, J. D. & Cheng, H.-J. Age-related changes in synaptic plasticity associated with mossy fiber terminal integration during adult neurogenesis. eNeuro https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0030-20.2020 (2020).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Weisz, V. I. & Argibay, P. F. Neurogenesis interferes with the retrieval of remote memories: Forgetting in neurocomputational terms. Cognition 125, 13–25 (2012).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Denny, C. A. et al. Hippocampal memory traces are differentially modulated by experience, time, and adult neurogenesis. Neuron 83, 189–201 (2014).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Rangel, L. M., Quinn, L. K., Chiba, A. A., Gage, F. H. & Aimone, J. B. A hypothesis for temporal coding of young and mature granule cells. Front. Neurosci. 7, 75 (2013).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Temprana, S. G. et al. Delayed coupling to feedback inhibition during a critical period for the integration of adult-born granule cells. Neuron 85, 116–130 (2014).

Article PubMed PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar

Tuncdemir, S. N., Lacefield, C. O. & Hen, R. Contributions of adult neurogenesis to dentate gyrus network activity and computations. Behav. Brain Res. 374, 112112 (2019).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Restivo, L., Niibori, Y., Mercaldo, V., Josselyn, S. A. & Frankland, P. W. Development of adult-generated cell connectivity with excitatory and inhibitory cell populations in the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 35, 10600–10612 (2015).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Barnes, C. A., McNaughton, B. L., Mizumori, S. J., Leonard, B. W. & Lin, L. H. Comparison of spatial and temporal characteristics of neuronal activity in sequential stages of hippocampal processing. Prog. Brain Res. 83, 287–300 (1990).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Amaral, D. G., Ishizuka, N. & Claiborne, B. Neurons, numbers and the hippocampal network. Prog. Brain Res. 83, 1–11 (1990).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Ishizuka, N., Weber, J. & Amaral, D. G. Organization of intrahippocampal projections originating from CA3 pyramidal cells in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 295, 580–623 (1990).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Amaral, D. G. Emerging principles of intrinsic hippocampal organization. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 3, 225–229 (1993).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Murphy, T. H., Worley, P. F. & Baraban, J. M. L-type voltage-sensitive calcium channels mediate synaptic activation of immediate early genes. Neuron 7, 625–635 (1991).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Barth, A. L., Gerkin, R. C. & Dean, K. L. Alteration of neuronal firing properties after in vivo experience in a FosGFP transgenic mouse. J. Neurosci. 24, 6466–6475 (2004).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Bourne, A. R. et al. Olfactory cues increase avoidance behavior and induce Fos expression in the amygdala, hippocampus and prefrontal cortex of socially defeated mice. Behav. Brain Res. 256, 188–196 (2013).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Wheeler, A. L. et al. Identification of a functional connectome for long-term fear memory in mice. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9, e1002853 (2013).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Scott, G. A. et al. Disrupted neurogenesis in germ-free mice: Effects of age and sex. Front Cell Dev Biol 8, 407 (2020).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Dana, H. et al. High-performance calcium sensors for imaging activity in neuronal populations and microcompartments. Nat. Methods 16, 649–657 (2019).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Muir, J. et al. Ventral hippocampal afferents to nucleus accumbens encode both latent vulnerability and stress-induced susceptibility. Biol. Psychiatry 88, 843–854 (2020).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Thompson, E. H. et al. Removal of perineuronal nets disrupts recall of a remote fear memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 115, 607–612 (2018).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Reichelt, A. C., Hare, D. J., Bussey, T. J. & Saksida, L. M. Perineuronal Nets: Plasticity, protection, and therapeutic potential. Trends Neurosci. 42, 458–470 (2019).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Shi, W. et al. Perineuronal nets protect long-term memory by limiting activity-dependent inhibition from parvalbumin interneurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1902680116 (2019).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Smith, C. C. et al. Differential regulation of perineuronal nets in the brain and spinal cord with exercise training. Brain Res. Bull. 111, 20–26 (2015).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Yamada, J. & Jinno, S. Spatio-temporal differences in perineuronal net expression in the mouse hippocampus, with reference to parvalbumin. Neuroscience 253, 368–379 (2013).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Cope, E. C. & Gould, E. Adult neurogenesis, Glia, and the extracellular matrix. Cell Stem Cell 24, 690–705 (2019).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Niibori, Y. et al. Suppression of adult neurogenesis impairs population coding of similar contexts in hippocampal CA3 region. Nat. Commun. 3, 1253 (2012).

Article ADS PubMed CAS Google Scholar

Kronenberg, G. et al. Physical exercise prevents age-related decline in precursor cell activity in the mouse dentate gyrus. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 1505–1513 (2006).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Reichelt, A. C. et al. Age-dependent and region-specific alteration of parvalbumin neurons, perineuronal nets and microglia in the mouse prefrontal cortex and hippocampus following obesogenic diet consumption. Sci. Rep. 11, 5593 (2021).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Mix, A., Hoppenrath, K. & Funke, K. Reduction in cortical parvalbumin expression due to intermittent theta-burst stimulation correlates with maturation of the perineuronal nets in young rats. Dev. Neurobiol. 75, 1–11 (2014).

Article PubMed CAS Google Scholar

Venturino, A. et al. Microglia enable mature perineuronal nets disassembly upon anesthetic ketamine exposure or 60-Hz light entrainment in the healthy brain. Cell Rep. 36, 109313 (2021).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Carstens, K. E. et al. Perineuronal net degradation rescues CA2 plasticity in a mouse model of Rett syndrome. J. Clin. Invest. 131, e137221 (2021).

Article CAS PubMed Central Google Scholar

Hall, J., Thomas, K. L. & Everitt, B. J. Cellular imaging of zif268 expression in the hippocampus and amygdala during contextual and cued fear memory retrieval: Selective activation of hippocampal CA1 neurons during the recall of contextual memories. J. Neurosci. 21, 2186–2193 (2001).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Jimenez, J. C. et al. Contextual fear memory retrieval by correlated ensembles of ventral CA1 neurons. Nat. Commun. 11, 3492 (2020).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Goshen, I. et al. Dynamics of retrieval strategies for remote memories. Cell 147, 678–689 (2011).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Iwasaki, S. & Ikegaya, Y. Contextual fear memory retrieval is vulnerable to hippocampal noise. Cereb. Cortex 31, 785–794 (2021).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Wiskott, L., Rasch, M. J. & Kempermann, G. A functional hypothesis for adult hippocampal neurogenesis: Avoidance of catastrophic interference in the dentate gyrus. Hippocampus 16, 329–343 (2006).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Kim, C. K. et al. Simultaneous fast measurement of circuit dynamics at multiple sites across the mammalian brain. Nat. Methods 13, 325–328 (2016).

Article PubMed PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar

Martianova, E., Aronson, S. & Proulx, C. D. Multi-fiber photometry to record neural activity in freely-moving animals. J. Vis. Exp. https://doi.org/10.3791/60278 (2019).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Patel, A. A., McAlinden, N., Mathieson, K. & Sakata, S. Simultaneous electrophysiology and fiber photometry in freely behaving mice. Front. Neurosci. 14, 148 (2020).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Reichenbach, A. et al. Metabolic sensing in AgRP neurons integrates homeostatic state with dopamine signalling in the striatum. Elife 11, e72668 (2022).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Morel, C. et al. Midbrain projection to the basolateral amygdala encodes anxiety-like but not depression-like behaviors. Nat. Commun. 13, 1352 (2022).

Article ADS CAS Google Scholar

Siciliano, C. A. & Tye, K. M. Leveraging calcium imaging to illuminate circuit dysfunction in addiction. Alcohol 74, 47–63 (2019).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Lerner, T. N. et al. Intact-brain analyses reveal distinct information carried by SNc dopamine subcircuits. Cell 162, 635–647 (2015).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Romberg, C. et al. Depletion of perineuronal nets enhances recognition memory and long-term depression in the perirhinal cortex. J. Neurosci. 33, 7057–7065 (2013).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Ben Abdallah, N.M.-B., Slomianka, L., Vyssotski, A. L. & Lipp, H.-P. Early age-related changes in adult hippocampal neurogenesis in C57 mice. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 151–161 (2008).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Khoo, G. H., Lin, Y.-T., Tsai, T.-C. & Hsu, K.-S. Perineuronal nets restrict the induction of long-term depression in the mouse Hippocampal CA1 region. Mol. Neurobiol. 56, 6436–6450 (2019).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Wiegert, J. S. & Oertner, T. G. Long-term depression triggers the selective elimination of weakly integrated synapses. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 110, E4510–E4519 (2013).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Berdugo-Vega, G. et al. Increasing neurogenesis refines hippocampal activity rejuvenating navigational learning strategies and contextual memory throughout life. Nat. Commun. 11, 135 (2020).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Keiser, A. A. et al. Sex differences in context fear generalization and recruitment of hippocampus and amygdala during retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 397–407 (2017).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Gruene, T. M., Flick, K., Stefano, A., Shea, S. D. & Shansky, R. M. Sexually divergent expression of active and passive conditioned fear responses in rats. Elife 4, 11352 (2015).

Article Google Scholar

Maren, S., De Oca, B. & Fanselow, M. S. Sex differences in hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats: Positive correlation between LTP and contextual learning. Brain Res. 661, 25–34 (1994).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Duarte-Guterman, P., Yagi, S., Chow, C. & Galea, L. A. M. Hippocampal learning, memory, and neurogenesis: Effects of sex and estrogens across the lifespan in adults. Horm. Behav. 74, 37–52 (2015).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Chow, C., Epp, J. R., Lieblich, S. E., Barha, C. K. & Galea, L. A. M. Sex differences in neurogenesis and activation of new neurons in response to spatial learning and memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1236–1250 (2013).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Mahmoud, R., Wainwright, S. R. & Galea, L. A. M. Sex hormones and adult hippocampal neurogenesis: Regulation, implications, and potential mechanisms. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 41, 129–152 (2016).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Kitamura, T. & Inokuchi, K. Role of adult neurogenesis in hippocampal-cortical memory consolidation. Mol. Brain 7, 13 (2014).

Article PubMed PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar

Wei, L., Meaney, M. J., Duman, R. S. & Kaffman, A. Affiliative behavior requires juvenile, but not adult neurogenesis. J. Neurosci. 31, 14335–14345 (2011).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Cushman, J. D. et al. Juvenile neurogenesis makes essential contributions to adult brain structure and plays a sex-dependent role in fear memories. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 6, 3 (2012).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Kuhn, H. G., Dickinson-Anson, H. & Gage, F. H. Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the adult rat: Age-related decrease of neuronal progenitor proliferation. J. Neurosci. 16, 2027–2033 (1996).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Epp, J. R., Barker, J. M. & Galea, L. A. M. Running wild: Neurogenesis in the hippocampus across the lifespan in wild and laboratory-bred Norway rats. Hippocampus 19, 1040–1049 (2009).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Encinas, J. M. et al. Division-coupled astrocytic differentiation and age-related depletion of neural stem cells in the adult hippocampus. Cell Stem Cell 8, 566–579 (2011).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Yang, T.-T. et al. Aging and exercise affect hippocampal neurogenesis via different mechanisms. PLoS ONE 10, e0132152 (2015).

Article PubMed PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar

Mosher, K. I. & Schaffer, D. V. Influence of hippocampal niche signals on neural stem cell functions during aging. Cell Tissue Res. 371, 115–124 (2018).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Moreno-Jiménez, E. P., Terreros-Roncal, J., Flor-García, M., Rábano, A. & Llorens-Martín, M. Evidences for adult hippocampal neurogenesis in humans. J. Neurosci. 41, 2541–2553 (2021).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Tran, L. M., Josselyn, S. A., Richards, B. A. & Frankland, P. W. Forgetting at biologically realistic levels of neurogenesis in a large-scale hippocampal model. Behav. Brain Res. 376, 112180 (2019).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Eriksson, P. S. et al. Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nat. Med. 4, 1313–1317 (1998).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Boldrini, M. et al. Antidepressants increase neural progenitor cells in the human hippocampus. Neuropsychopharmacology 34, 2376–2389 (2009).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Knoth, R. et al. Murine features of neurogenesis in the human hippocampus across the lifespan from 0 to 100 years. PLoS ONE 5, e8809 (2010).

Article ADS PubMed PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar

Epp, J. R., Beasley, C. L. & Galea, L. A. Increased hippocampal neurogenesis and p21 expression in depression: Dependent on antidepressants, sex, age, and antipsychotic exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 2297–2306 (2013).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Spalding, K. L. et al. Dynamics of hippocampal neurogenesis in adult humans. Cell 153, 1219–1227 (2013).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Boldrini, M. et al. Human hippocampal neurogenesis persists throughout aging. Cell Stem Cell 22, 589-599.e5 (2018).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Moreno-Jiménez, E. P. et al. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis is abundant in neurologically healthy subjects and drops sharply in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Nat. Med. 25, 554–560 (2019).

Article PubMed CAS Google Scholar

Tobin, M. K. et al. Human hippocampal neurogenesis persists in aged adults and Alzheimer's disease patients. Cell Stem Cell 24, 974-982.e3 (2019).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Sorrells, S. F. et al. Human hippocampal neurogenesis drops sharply in children to undetectable levels in adults. Nature 555, 377–381 (2018).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Sorrells, S. F. et al. Positive controls in adults and children support that very few, if any, new neurons are born in the adult human hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 41, 2554–2565 (2021).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Franjic, D. et al. Transcriptomic taxonomy and neurogenic trajectories of adult human, macaque, and pig hippocampal and entorhinal cells. Neuron 110, 452-469.e14 (2022).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Santarelli, L. et al. Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science 301, 805–809 (2003).

Article ADS CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Ernst, C., Olson, A. K., Pinel, J. P. J., Lam, R. W. & Christie, B. R. Antidepressant effects of exercise: Evidence for an adult-neurogenesis hypothesis?. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 31, 84–92 (2006).

PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Hill, A. S., Sahay, A. & Hen, R. Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to reduce anxiety and depression-like behaviors. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 2368–2378 (2015).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Marques, A. A. et al. Gender differences in the neurobiology of anxiety: Focus on adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Neural Plast. 2016, 5026713 (2016).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Gomes-Leal, W. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and affective disorders: New neurons for psychic well-being. Front. Neurosci. 15, 594448 (2021).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Burghardt, N. S., Park, E. H., Hen, R. & Fenton, A. A. Adult-born hippocampal neurons promote cognitive flexibility in mice. Hippocampus 22, 1795–1808 (2012).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Anacker, C. & Hen, R. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and cognitive flexibility: Linking memory and mood. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 335–346 (2017).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Berdugo-Vega, G., Lee, C.-C., Garthe, A., Kempermann, G. & Calegari, F. Adult-born neurons promote cognitive flexibility by improving memory precision and indexing. Hippocampus 31, 1068–1079 (2021).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Aimone, J. B., Deng, W. & Gage, F. H. Resolving new memories: A critical look at the dentate gyrus, adult neurogenesis, and pattern separation. Neuron 70, 589–596 (2011).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Yagi, S., Chow, C., Lieblich, S. E. & Galea, L. A. M. Sex and strategy use matters for pattern separation, adult neurogenesis, and immediate early gene expression in the hippocampus. Hippocampus 26, 87–101 (2016).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Jessberger, S. et al. Dentate gyrus-specific knockdown of adult neurogenesis impairs spatial and object recognition memory in adult rats. Learn. Mem. 16, 147–154 (2009).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Arruda-Carvalho, M., Sakaguchi, M., Akers, K. G., Josselyn, S. A. & Frankland, P. W. Posttraining ablation of adult-generated neurons degrades previously acquired memories. J. Neurosci. 31, 15113–15127 (2011).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Cope, E. C. & Gould, E. Adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus: A role in learning and memory. In Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference 345–358 (Elsevier, 2017).

Chapter Google Scholar

Ueno, H. et al. Juvenile stress induces behavioral change and affects perineuronal net formation in juvenile mice. BMC Neurosci. 19, 41 (2018).

Article PubMed PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar

Yu, Z. et al. Decreased density of perineuronal net in prelimbic cortex is linked to depressive-like behavior in young-aged rats. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 13, 4 (2020).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Koskinen, M.-K., van Mourik, Y., Smit, A. B., Riga, D. & Spijker, S. From stress to depression: development of extracellular matrix-dependent cognitive impairment following social stress. Sci. Rep. 10, 17308 (2020).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Anderson, M. D. et al. ChABC infusions into medial prefrontal cortex, but not posterior parietal cortex, improve the performance of rats tested on a novel, challenging delay in the touchscreen TUNL task. Learn. Mem. 27, 222–235 (2020).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Gogolla, N., Caroni, P., Lüthi, A. & Herry, C. Perineuronal nets protect fear memories from erasure. Science 325, 1258–1261 (2009).

Article ADS CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Happel, M. F. K. et al. Enhanced cognitive flexibility in reversal learning induced by removal of the extracellular matrix in auditory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 111, 2800–2805 (2014).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Lesnikova, A. et al. Perineuronal net receptor PTPσ regulates retention of memories. Front. Synaptic Neurosci. 13, 672475 (2021).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Sorg, B. A. et al. Casting a wide net: Role of perineuronal nets in neural plasticity. J. Neurosci. 36, 11459–11468 (2016).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Carulli, D. & Verhaagen, J. An extracellular perspective on CNS maturation: Perineuronal nets and the control of plasticity. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 22, 2434 (2021).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Lopes, G. et al. Bonsai: An event-based framework for processing and controlling data streams. Front. Neuroinform. 9, 7 (2015).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Meng, C. et al. Spectrally resolved fiber photometry for multi-component analysis of brain circuits. Neuron 98, 707-717.e4 (2018).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Moda-Sava, R. N. et al. Sustained rescue of prefrontal circuit dysfunction by antidepressant-induced spine formation. Science 364, 6436 (2019).

Article CAS Google Scholar

Berg, S. et al. ilastik: Interactive machine learning for (bio)image analysis. Nat. Methods 16, 1226–1232 (2019).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Sigal, Y. M., Bae, H., Bogart, L. J., Hensch, T. K. & Zhuang, X. Structural maturation of cortical perineuronal nets and their perforating synapses revealed by superresolution imaging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 116, 7071–7076 (2019).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Ho, J., Tumkaya, T., Aryal, S., Choi, H. & Claridge-Chang, A. Moving beyond P values: Data analysis with estimation graphics. Nat. Methods 16, 565–566 (2019).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Download references

Funding for this study was provided by an NSERC Discovery Grant (RGPIN-2018-05135) to JRE and a Brain Canada Early Career research capacity building Grant (4709) to JRE. DJT received a fellowship from the Canadian Open Neuroscience Platform. GAS received PDF fellowships from NSERC, the Hotchkiss Brain Institute and the Cumming School of Medicine.

These authors contributed equally: Alexandria Evans and Dylan J. Terstege.

Department of Cell Biology and Anatomy, Hotchkiss Brain Institute, Cumming School of Medicine, HMRB 162, Health Sciences Centre, University of Calgary, 3330 Hospital Drive NW, Calgary, AB, T2N 4N1, Canada

Alexandria Evans, Dylan J. Terstege, Gavin A. Scott, Mio Tsutsui & Jonathan R. Epp

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

A.E., D.J.T., and J.R.E. conceived and designed the experiments. A.E., D.J.T., and G.A.S. performed the experiments. G.A.S. and M.T. performed the surgical procedures. A.E, D.J.T., G.A.S., and J.R.E. performed the histological procedures. A.E., D.J.T., and J.R.E. conducted the analyses. A.E., D.J.T., and J.R.E. wrote the paper.

Correspondence to Jonathan R. Epp.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and Permissions

Evans, A., Terstege, D.J., Scott, G.A. et al. Neurogenesis mediated plasticity is associated with reduced neuronal activity in CA1 during context fear memory retrieval. Sci Rep 12, 7016 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-10947-w

Download citation

Received: 10 January 2022

Accepted: 14 April 2022

Published: 29 April 2022

DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-10947-w

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

SHARE